翻訳と辞書
Words near each other
・ Sentenac-de-Sérou
・ Sentence
・ Sentence (law)
・ Sentence (linguistics)
・ Sentence (logic)
・ Sentence (music)
・ Sentence arrangement
・ Sentence boundary disambiguation
・ Sentence clause structure
・ Sentence completion tests
・ Sentence diagram
・ Sentence extraction
・ Sentence function
・ Sentence length
・ Sentence of Death
Sentence processing
・ Sentence Review Commission
・ Sentence spacing
・ Sentence spacing in digital media
・ Sentence spacing in language and style guides
・ Sentence spacing studies
・ Sentence word
・ Sentence-final particle
・ Sentenced
・ Sentenced discography
・ Sentenced for Life
・ Sentenced for Life (1960 film)
・ Sentenced to Death (film)
・ Sentenced to Prism
・ Sentences


Dictionary Lists
翻訳と辞書 辞書検索 [ 開発暫定版 ]
スポンサード リンク

Sentence processing : ウィキペディア英語版
Sentence processing

Sentence processing takes place whenever a reader or listener processes a language utterance, either in isolation or in the context of a conversation or a text.
Many studies of the human language comprehension process have focused on reading of single utterances (sentences) without context. Extensive research has shown, however, that language comprehension is affected also by context preceding a given utterance, as well as many other factors.
==Ambiguity and sentence comprehension==
Sentence comprehension has to deal with ambiguity in spoken and written utterances, for example lexical, structural, and semantic ambiguities. Ambiguity is ubiquitous, but people usually resolve it so effortlessly that they don't even notice it. For example, the sentence ''Time flies like an arrow'' has (at least) the interpretations ''Time moves as quickly as an arrow'', ''A special kind of fly, called time fly, likes arrows'' and ''Measure the speed of flies like you would measure the speed of an arrow''. Usually, readers will be only aware of the first interpretation.

Instances of ambiguity can be classified as local or global ambiguities. A sentence is globally ambiguous if it has two distinct interpretations. Examples are sentences like ''Someone shot the servant of the actress who was on the balcony'' (was it the servant or the actress who was on the balcony?) or ''The cop chased the criminal with a fast car'' (did the cop or the criminal have a fast car?). Comprehenders may have a preferential interpretation for either of these cases, but syntactically and semantically, neither of the possible interpretations can be ruled out.
Local ambiguities persist only for a short amount of time as an utterance is heard or written and are resolved during the course of the utterance, so that the complete utterance has only one interpretation. Examples include sentences like ''The critic wrote the book was enlightening'', which is ambiguous when ''The critic wrote the book'' has been encountered, but ''was enlightening'' remains to be processed. At this point, the sentence could either end, stating that the critic is the author of the book, or it could go on to clarify that the critic wrote something about a book. The ambiguity ends at ''was enlightening'', which determines that the second alternative is correct.
When readers process a local ambiguity, they settle on one of the possible interpretations immediately, without waiting to hear or read more words that might help decide which interpretation is correct (this behaviour is called ''incremental processing''). If they are surprised by the turn the sentence really takes, processing is slowed. This is visible for example in reading times. Locally ambiguous sentences therefore have been used as test cases to investigate the influence of a number of different factors on human sentence processing. If a factor helps readers to avoid difficulty, it is clear that this factor plays a factor in sentence processing.

抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)
ウィキペディアで「Sentence processing」の詳細全文を読む



スポンサード リンク
翻訳と辞書 : 翻訳のためのインターネットリソース

Copyright(C) kotoba.ne.jp 1997-2016. All Rights Reserved.